Visual function,performance,and processing of basketball players vs.sedentary individuals
2020-12-18JesVerRimundoJimnezDvidrdensBetrizRedondoJosAntonioGrc
Jesús Ver*,Rimundo JiménezDvid Cárdens,Betriz RedondoJosé Antonio Grcí
aDepartment of Optics,Faculty of Science,University of Granada,Granada 18071,Spain bDepartment of Physical Education and Sport,Faculty of Sport Science,University of Granada,Granada 18071,Spain cSport and Health University Mixed Institute,University of Granada,Granada 18071,Spain
Abstract Background:Athletes tend to have better visuo-motor performance than do sedentary individuals.However,several basic visual-function and perceptual parameters remain unexplored to date.In this study,we investigated whether differences exist in visual function,performance,and processing between basketball players and individuals without a sport-involvement background.Methods:A total of 33 healthy men with no visual impairment or pathology were divided into 2 groups,depending on the involvement in sport(semi-professional basketball players and sedentary individuals).We tested their baseline heart-rate variability in the resting position apart from subjective questionnaires to determine their physical fitness level,and we checked their visual function,performance,and processing through an extended battery of optometric tests.Results:The 2 groups differed in resting heart-rate variability parameters(p<0.001),con firming their dissimilarities in regular time practising sports per week.The basketball players showed a closer breakpoint and recovery nearpoint of convergence,a higher fusional-vergence rate,better discriminability halos,and better eye-hand coordination(all p values<0.05).Conclusion:These results show evidence that athletes,basketball players in this case,exhibit better performance in several visual abilities in comparison to a group of individuals without sporting backgrounds,suggesting an improvement due to the systematic involvement of those skills during basketball practice.
Keywords:Exercise;Fitness level;Health;Heart rate variability;Visual skills;Team sport
1.Introduction
Athletes need to gather a great amount of information,mainly visual,swiftly from the environment in order to execute appropriate motor tasks.1There is evidence that athletes develop peculiar mechanisms of occipital neural synchronization during visuo-spatial demands,showing better visuo-motor performance compared to non-athletes.2Previous studies tend to indicate that athletes present better visual skills than do sedentary individuals,but this issue is far from being settled.3Several studies have questioned whether visual skills in athletes are innate or whether they are improved with systematic sport practice.4In this context,it has been established that constant practice and sport vision-training programs help to improve certain visual abilities,while the innate contributions seem to be insignificant.5,6
Previous investigation suggests that particular sets of visual skills are sport-dependent because each discipline has differing visual needs and demands.7The visual information during basketball,as a dynamic sport,comes from the position of the ball and player.Thus,basic visual function based on good optical quality,oculomotor coordination,binocular and accommodative function,or stereopsis,are crucial to success in ball games and particularly in basketball.8In addition,a player's performance depends on cognitive capabilities and the visuo-motor reaction times.9
Nevertheless,it has not been clarified whether athletes'superiority is due to basic visual function or perceptual and cognitive skills.10An increasing body of knowledge supports a multidimensional approach,considering visual,perceptual,and cognitive factors to characterise expertise.11Although some studies have concluded that athletes possess better visual function than do sedentary individuals,12few investigations have used an extended optometric battery of tests in a specificsport discipline and,thus,no solid conclusions have been drawn to date.For example,differences between professional volleyball players and a control group have been reported for saccadic eye movements and facility of ocular accommodation,13as well as better near stereoacuity in young baseball/softball players in comparison to non-ball players.14By contrast,Paulus et al.15found that stereopsis in soccer players was similar to that of individuals without soccer backgrounds.On the other hand,visual information processing also plays a fundamental role in sport performance,permitting a precise decision-making process in a certain amount of time.16Several studies have shown that athletes have better peripheral awareness and ability to track a moving target,and they have a different strategy in the treatment of visual information,among other advantages,than do non-athletes or less experienced players.6,17,18Specifically,in a recent study,Mangine et al.16found a relationship between faster visual-tracking speed and better basketball-specificperformance in players in the National Basketball Association.
Considering the previous literature and the ongoing debate concerning the differences in visual function and perceptual abilities between athletes and the sedentary population,in the present study we investigated the basic visual function and perceptual visual skills in a specificsport discipline,basketball in our case,in order to provide more knowledge in this regard.Therefore,we tested several parameters related to the basic visual function,such as accommodative response,near point of convergence,near and far fusional vergences,and near and far stereoacuity.Regarding perceptual abilities,we also assessed visual performance by visual-discrimination capacity and visual-information processing by visual-reaction time and eye-hand coordination.
Additionally,exercise practice has been demonstrated to improve the autonomic balance through an increase in parasympathetic and a decrease in sympathetic control of heart rate(HR).19,20Heart rate variability(HRV)analysis with the time and frequency domains permits assessment of the state of the autonomic nervous system,which indirectly re flects fitness level.21To check the differences in physical exercise involvement between athletes and individuals without a sport background,we measured HRV at rest and compiled subjective report data.We hypothesised that inherent visual involvement during systematic basketball practice can improve both the basic visual function and the main perceptual visual parameters involved.The answer to our research question can have theoretical and practical consequences for basketball performance and training protocols.
2.Methods
2.1.Participants and ethical approval
The study included a total of 33 male university students,of whom 18 belonged to different basketball teams in a regional league,and 15 had no history of sporting activity(age:23.28±2.37 years and 22.27±2.09 years,mean±SD,respectively)(Table 1).Participants were asked about the type of sport activities that they were engaged in apart from basketball and the amount of time dedicated to each sport discipline.From the basketball group,15/18 reported that they practiced strength training in addition to the basketball sessions,and the other 3 were engaged only in basketball.All basketball players had been playing at competitive levels for at least 7 years(10.24±2.27).Regarding the sedentary participants,8 individuals reported occasionally practicing team sports,and the rest of them(7 participants)were not involved in any physical activity.This study was conducted according to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association(Declaration of Helsinki),and permission was provided by the Institutional Review Board of University of Granada.All participants gave written informed consent prior to the study.
Admission criteria included:(1)being healthy;(2)practising 6 h or more of moderate exercise per week for the athlete group and 1 h or less of exercise per week for the non-athlete group;(3)not presenting any ocular pathology;(4)not taking any medication;(5)presenting static monocular(in both eyes)and binocular visual acuity ≤ 0 logMAR(≥ 20/20);(6)having a corrected refractive error≤3.5 diopters for myopia and hyperopia and≤1.5 diopters of astigmatism and being contact lens wearers;and(7)scoring less than 25 on the Conlon Survey,22which assesses visual discomfort,and less than 21 on the Convergence Insufficiency Symptom Survey23(Table 1).All participants were instructed to avoid alcohol consumption and vigorous exercise 24 h before the experiment session,to sleep for at least 7 h,not to consume caffeine beverages or other stimulants in the 3 h prior to testing,and to follow their regular diets but not to eat 2 h prior to testing.
Table 1Anthropometrical and visual characteristics and visual symptomatology of the 33 participants included in this study by groups(mean±SD,range).
2.2.Test procedures and apparatus
2.2.1.HRV measure and analysis
To ensure differences in physical involvement,we measured HRV.24A number of studies have concluded that endurance training enhances vagal tone in athletes,which may contribute in part to lower the resting HR.19Thus,before the visual examination,the participant was asked to lie in a supine position in a quiet room for 6 min.The HR was monitored by using a Polar RS800CX wrist device(Polar Electro Oy.,Kempele,Finland)set to measure both the HR and HRV.The time series of HRV was taken from the electrocardiogram,identifying the occurrence of each R wave(belonging to the QRS complex)and calculating the time lapse between 2 consecutive R waves.21Subsequently,the data were transferred to the Polar ProTrainer Software(Polar Electro),and each downloaded R-R interval(inter-beat R wave to R wave) file was then further analysed using the Kubios HRV Analysis Software 2.0(The Biomedical Signal and Medical Imaging Analysis Group,Department of Applied Physics,University of Kuopia,Finland).25R-R intervals that differed more than 25%from the previous and subsequent R-R intervals were excluded.The removed R-R intervals were replaced by conventional spline interpolation so that the length of the data did not change.In this study,the parameters used to analyse HRV within the time domain were the mean R-R interval and the root-mean-square difference of successive normal R-R intervals(rMSSD),and within the frequency domain were the low-frequency and high-frequency components in normalized units,which are established to be between 0.04 Hz and 0.15 Hz and 0.15 Hz and 0.4 Hz,respectively.19
2.2.2.Ocular and visual examination
Ocular parameters related to ocular refraction,accommodative and binocular function,visual performance,and visual information processing were examined.All tests were conducted under photopic illuminance conditions(152.4±2.45 lux),with the exception of visual discrimination in scotopic illuminance conditions(~0 lux),which were quantified in the corneal plane with an illuminance meter T-10(Konica Minolta,Tokyo,Japan).
2.2.3.Ocular refraction
Monocular and binocular visual acuity was determined by using a computerized monitor with the logarithmic letters chart test employing the Bailey-Lovie design(POLA VistaVision,DMD Med Tech,Torino,Italy)at a distance of 5 m.
Ocular refraction consisted of an objective refraction with non-cyclopegic retinoscopy while the participant maintained a fixed gaze on a distant non-accommodative target and, finally,each participant underwent a full monocular and binocular subjective refraction,using an endpoint criterion of maximum plus consistent with best vision.
2.2.4.Accommodation,binocular,and oculomotor parameters
All tests were conducted with the best correction,following the recommendations given by Scheiman and Wick.26
The accommodative response,measured by the monocular estimate method of retinoscopy,was carried out by very brie fly interposing,in front of 1 eye at a time,convergent or divergent lenses until neutralizing the re flex found in the horizontal meridian,while the participant read a test close-up with 0.18 logMAR(20/30)letters.
The near point of convergence was evaluated by the pushup technique using an accommodative target.A 0.18 logMAR(20/30)single letter on the fixation stick was used as the target.The target was moved closer until the participant experienced constant diplopia on the stick(breakpoint).Then we asked the patient to move it away from the eye until single vision was restored(recovery point).
Near and distance negative and positive vergence amplitudes were measured.The negative fusional vergence was measured first to avoid affecting the vergence-recovery value because of excessive stimulation of convergence.A gradually increasing prism bar was introduced in the dominant eye while the patient fixed the gaze on a column of the Snellen optotype,corresponding to the highest visual acuity at 40 cm and 6 m fixation,respectively.When the prism caused the patient to experience double vision,the amount of prism(breakpoint)was recorded.The prism power was then reduced until the double images could be fused again(recovery point).
Static far stereo acuity was tested using the Stereo D6/D8(POLA VistaVision)at 5 m away using a polarizing viewer.This test presents a range from a maximum of 300 s of arc to a minimum of 10 s of arc,and only 1 circle from 5 possible choices had crossed disparity.The participant was asked to identify which circle seemed to be at a different distance with respect to the other 2(at near)or 4(at distance).Static near stereo acuity was measured using the Randot Stereotest Circles(Stereo Optical,Chicago,IL,USA)at a distance of 40 cm.Within each of 10 targets,there were 3 circles.This test presents a range from a maximum of 400 s of arc to a minimum of 20 s of arc.The level of stereoacuity was recorded as the last series of targets answered correctly.
To test facility of accommodation,the Hart chart was read under binocular conditions.This procedure presents blur and vergence-related visual feedback and function in an interactive manner.27Participants were instructed to read alternatively 1 letter from the distance Hart chart(5 m)in primary position and then shift their focus to the near Hart chart(40 cm)placed 30°inferiorly,and so forth,across the lines of letters as rapidly as possible.The number of cycles completed in 60 s was determined as well as the number of errors made.
2.2.5.Visual performance
We evaluated the visual-discrimination capacity,quantifying the visual disturbances perceived by the participant by using a visual test conducted by the software Halo,version 1.0(freeware software,University of Granada,Granada,Spain).28The participant's task consisted of detecting luminous peripheral stimuli around a central high-luminance stimulus over a dark background.All of the stimuli were achromatic.The distance from the observer to the test monitor(1280×1024 LCD screen)was 2.5 m,and the test was performed binocularly.The size of the stimulus was 39 pixels for the radius of the central stimulus and 1 pixel for the peripheral stimulus,subtending 0.61 degrees and 0.02 degrees,respectively,from the observer's position.The monitor showed 72 peripheral stimuli around the central one,distributed along 18 semi-axes.Each of the 72 stimuli was presented twice.After a 3-min adaption period to the darkness of the monitor background,there was 1-min adaptation to the main stimulus,and then the participant was randomly presented with peripheral stimuli around the central stimulus.On detecting peripheral spots,the participant pressed a button on the mouse to store this information for subsequent treatment and calculation of the visual disturbance index(VDI)after the test was concluded.The VDI takes values from 0 to 1.A greater value indicates a greater contribution of visual disturbances,such as glare or visual halos around the luminous stimuli and,therefore,poorer discrimination capacity.
2.2.6.Visual-information processing
The Wayne Saccadic fixator(Wayne Engineering,Skokie,IL,USA)was used for evaluating visual reaction time.This apparatus consists of a 29-inch square panel containing 33 redlight switches.A computer chip generates a variety of patterns of light to which an individual responds by pushing the illuminated switch to extinguish the light.A great variety of display patterns,speeds,and situations can be programmed.The Sports Vision Release/Locate Reaction Time program,used to test visual reaction time,was performed 3 times after familiarization.The test instructions consisted of pressing the start/reset button,holding the button down until a signal was heard(liberalization time),releasing the button,and pressing the illuminated light/button on the saccadic fixator(localization time).Just one light was used,and it appeared in a random position each time.Two scores were given to each trial for the time of liberalization and location(milliseconds).29,30
To test eye-hand coordination,we used a standardized test developed by Dr.Jack Gardner,which employs the Wayne Saccadic Fixator;it jointly takes into account the proaction(time period in which each light stays lit until the button is pressed)and reaction times(preset amount of time in which each light stays lit before automatically switching to another light,regardless of whether the button is pressed)for accurate and repeatable rapid testing.The lights start moving automatically at the preset speed(60 lights/min).For each correct response,the speed increases.At the end of the preset time(30 s),the display shows the number of correct responses,the average speed,and the final speed in lights/min.The score is the product of the number of lights scored and the final speed of presentation of the lights.29
hree measurements were taken in the monocular estimate method:near point of convergence(break and recovery points)and visual reaction time,and the mean value was used.When both eyes had to be measured independently,the order of the first eye was randomized,and if no statistical significance was found between eyes,the mean values were analysed.31
2.3.Statistical analysis
All variables tested were subjected to the Shapiro-Wilks test,showing a normal Gaussian distribution.Thus,to analyse the differences in visual function between basketball players and sedentary participants,a separatettest was performed for independent samples,and each variable was tested.We used the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparison.A value of 0.05 was adopted to determine significance.
3.Results
3.1.Sample manipulation check
Thettest for independent samples showed significant differences between basketball players and sedentary participants in the HR(beats/min)(t31=-7.07,p<0.001),R-R interval(ms)(t31=7.09,p<0.001),rMMSD(ms)(t31=5.14,p<0.001),low-frequency(nu)(t31=-5.28,p<0.001),and high-frequency(nu)(t31=5.27,p<0.001)(Table 2).Also,attest for independent samples was performed forhoursperweek ofexercise (t31=21.179,p<0.001)and for hours per week of basketball practice(t31=39.57,p<0.001)reported by participants.Hence,the 2 samples showed different fitness levels.
3.2.Visual parameters
Table 3 presents mean values±SD and significance for all parameters tested in this study.
Table 2HRV at rest and hours of exercise practice of the 33 participants included in this study by groups(mean±SD).
The analysis for fusional vergence showed that athletes had higherfarpositive fusionalvergence range (t31=2.69,p=0.011 for the breakpoint,andt31=3.02,p=0.005 for the recovery value).Regarding near positive fusional vergence,basketballplayersreached marginally significanthigher fusional vergence values for the breakpoint and recovery(t31=1.957,p=0.059,andt31=1.941,p=0.061,respectively).For the near point of convergence,closer breakpoints and recovery values were found for athletes(t31=-3.133,p=0.004 andt31=-2.615,p=0.014,respectively).Finally,the accommodative response,facility of accommodation,and static near and far stereo acuity yielded no significant differences(p>0.05)between groups(Table 3).
Participants without basketball backgrounds demonstrated significantly higher VDI than did the basketball players(t31=-3.282,p=0.003)(Table 3)(Fig.1,illustrating 2 participants in each experiment group).
Basketball players showed better scores for eye-hand coordination(t31=2.405,p=0.022).On the other hand,visual-reaction time revealed no differences for liberation and location times(p=0.784 andp=0.346,respectively)(Table 3).
Table 3Ocular parameters evaluated according to the measurement method and group analysed.Means±SD were calculated from the mean values of each participant(n=33,mean±SD).
4.Discussion
This investigation incorporates noteworthy findings in several categories:basketball players show a closer near point of convergence for breakpoint and recovery,a larger positive fusional vergence range,a better VD1 index(e.g.,lower scores),and higher scores in eye-hand coordination than do sedentary participants.
4.1.Accommodative and binocular function
In basketball,near/far visual changes are continual for ball interceptions,control,passing,and throwing the ball,as well as analysing the positioning of teammates and opponents,among others.16These types of actions promote a constant implication of the vergence/accommodative system,which could produce an effect comparable to visual therapy exercises.Exercises based on constant near/far changes in binocular viewing conditions are normally applied in optometry practice with the aim of normalising the accommodative and vergence system as well as their mutual interactions.32Notably,we found that basketball players present a closer near point of convergence and larger far positive fusional vergences in comparison with the sedentary group.Similar results were reported by Christenson and Winkelstein33and by Coffey and Reichow,34who found a closer near point of convergence and greater distance vergences ranges in athletes,respectively.On the contrary,no significant differences were found in the negative fusional vergences between groups in the current study.Similarly,Daum35demonstrated that visual training in young adults with normal binocularity has a significant and prolonged effect on positive vergences,while fusional negative vergences resisted change.These substantial differences between the convergence and divergence systems seem to be explained by their being controlled by different neural centres.It is also well known that visual therapy gives the best results in the treatment of convergence insufficiency,acting on the reduced positive fusional vergences and the receded near point of convergence.36
No statistical differences were found for near static stereopsis,but a tendency to present different values between groups was appreciable(38.33±20.72 for the basketball groupvs.86.33±128.71 for the sedentary group).Along this line,Boden et al.14found significant differences between baseball/softball players and non-ball players(25.5±11.9 and 56.2±60.7,respectively).Similarly,we found no differences for far static stereoacuity,and these results agree with those of Paulus et al.,15who made comparisons for far static and dynamic stereopsis in professional and amateur soccer playersvs.individuals without soccer backgrounds.The effect of specificeye exercises on stereoacuity seemed to be modest and has limited use in practical terms.37Therefore,we can expect that systematic basketball practice does not involve substantial stereopsis improvements.Also,as indicated by Paulus et al.,15stereopsis tests are not sensitive enough to reveal differences between groups,and further developments in test methodology of stereopsis are needed.
The accommodative system is controlled by the autonomic nervous system,and that system is more stable and efficient in athletes.13Therefore,we might expect a better accommodative function in basketball players,but we detected no significant differences in the accommodative responses between groups.We propose 2 possible explanations: firstly,the possible accommodation variations may be relatively smaller than monocular estimate method sensitivity(0.25 D),and more sensitive methods to test accommodative response would be necessary(e.g.,open- field autorefractor or wavefront sensors).Secondly,and perhaps most in fluential,the ball and players are moving mainly at medium to long distances,which do not require great accommodative effort.Therefore,accommodation enhancements,which require high accommodative stimulation in visual training,38are unlikely to be achieved only with regular basketball practice.
For its part,facility of accommodation in binocular conditions revealed no differences between groups.Little comparable work has been conducted,and only Jafarzadehpur et al.13found significant differences when they compared professional and intermediate female volleyball players with beginners and non-players,but those differences disappeared when professionals were compared to intermediates.However,the method of measuring used in their work is not clear.Other authors,using a methodology similar to that used in the present work,showed slightly better accommodative facility for a wide group of interceptive sports athletes than for non-athletes.39That study involved not only basketball players but also a great variety of sport modalities(e.g.,tennis,table tennis,baseball,volleyball,badminton),and the visual requirements for each discipline are substantially different.
Fig.1.Visual discrimination index(VDI)diagrams of 2 participants belonging to each experimental group(basketball n=11;sedentary n=4).Data in green represent correct responses:Numbers 1 and 2 indicate whether the stimulus was identified just once or both times,respectively.Red crosses indicate that no stimulus was identified in that position.
4.2.Visual performance
No study available has investigated visual performance in sports using the VDI.The present study reports better visual discrimination in athletes.It has importance because the perception of halos requires a longer time to recover after exposure to a high-luminance stimulus(e.g.,glare).40The glare phenomenon has great importance in basketball,and players are constantly exposed to glare due to illumination conditions in basketball courts.41The differences found between basketball players and sedentary individuals could be explained from the perspective that abilities involved during the game are inherently developed while playing the sport(e.g.,higher tolerance),6as occurs with the better selective attention demonstrated in international basketball players.42
4.3.Visual-information processing
Our results con firm that basketball players show better eyehand coordination than do individuals without basketball background.This study agrees with Houmourtzoglou et al.,42who found a similar result in members of a Greek national team,arguing different perceptual strategies from experts to novices in relevant and irrelevant cues.In relation to visual reaction times,previous studies have indicated that players in various disciplines(e.g.,water polo or soccer)had faster visual reaction times compared with novices or non-athletes,but no differences were found for basketball players,as in this study.43It may demonstrate that the nature of each sport strongly in fluences the development of visual skills with constant practice.Along the same line,other authors have stated that athletes have a speed of response similar to that of non-athletes but differ in the ability to detect pertinent cues associated with the higher level of expertise in sport.44
4.4.A plausible explanation
Previous works have supported the contention that differences between an athlete and a sedentary participant arise from visual-information processing and interpretation rather than from basic visual skills.2This study supports the hypothesis that athletes could present a combination of better basic visual function as well as perceptual and cognitive factors than do non-athletes,as explained by several authors.6,10,13,45However,this study does not elucidate whether there is an innate visual superiority in athletes or whether those superior skills are achieved due to the constant sport practice.In addition,the differing visual demands required in each sport discipline could in fluence the development of visual,perceptual,and cognitive skills.The vast majority of studies have reported that better visual skills would play a positive role in sport performance.This advantage in visuo-oculomotor abilities can lead to faster and better interceptive skills,motor responses,and decision making.39,46For example,a recent study indicates that visual tracking speed is related to a greater number of assists and steals and lower turnovers in National Basketball Association players.16Moreover,considering that our athletes never received specificvisual training,it is implied that basketball training in itself might be responsible for the differences in some visual capacities between basketball players and non-players,as explained by Alves et al.18for professional soccer players.
4.5.Implications for future research
Because of the great number of sport disciplines,further studies should be performed to analyse the differences presented to the visual system.We would like to encourage researchers to investigate whether visual training could be transferred to sport performance in the field environment.Some work is currently being performed in this area,5but more data are needed.It would be useful to explore the possible visual-function improvements with systematic sport practice in persons who have impaired visual function(e.g.,convergence insufficiency,vergence fusional dysfunction,etc.),as has been demonstrated in various health conditions.47
5.Conclusions
This study presents evidence of the differences between basketball players and sedentary individuals with respect to some skills in their visual function,performance,and processing.Both groups proved to have different sport backgrounds,as re flected by the HRV parameters and as indicated in the demographic questionnaire.In comparison to the control group,basketball players clearly present superiority in near point of convergence,positive fusional vergences,halo discriminability,and eye-hand coordination.Our results suggest that systematic basketball practice might be responsible for the development of certain visual abilities.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Ernesto Suarez and David Nesbitt for translating the text into English.We also acknowledge all the participants who sel flessly participated in this study.The study was funded in part by a Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness grant#DEP2013-48211-R.
Authors’contributions
JV conceived the study,participated in data collection,performed the statistical analysis,and drafted the manuscript;RJ conceived the study,coordinated and drafted the manuscript;DC coordinated and helped to draft the manuscript;BR participated in data collection and drafted the manuscript;JAG coordinated and drafted the manuscript.All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript,and agree with the order of presentation of the authors.
Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
杂志排行
Journal of Sport and Health Science的其它文章
- Reasons why older adults play sport:A systematic review
- Isokinetic trunk flexion-extension protocol to assess trunk muscle strength and endurance:Reliability,learning effect,and sex differences
- Walking pace and the risk of stroke:A meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies
- Application of network meta-analysis in the field of physical activity and health promotion
- Sedentary Behavior Research Network members support new Canadian 24-Hour Movement Guideline recommendations
- Are we really “screening”movement?The role of assessing movement quality in exercise settings